Josef Albers did wonderful work in his exploration of how we see color. His work can be Explore Albers' work in his book Interaction of Color. Many of our ideas build on his approach.
Notice in this woodblock print Matt made: the color of the x's are the same. They appear different because of the differing adjacent colors .
from Interaction of Color,
At the start of his chapter “The Relativity of Color”, Josef Albers describes a simple experiment using touch to experience relative water temperatures. He sets up three pots of water of three temperatures: warm, lukewarm, and cold. He asks that you put your two hands in the two outer pots, warm and cold, and after a period of time take those two hands and place them together in the pot of lukewarm water. He points out: the temperature sensations in your two hands will not be the same. The hand that had been in cold water will feel warm, the one that had been in warm water will feel cold. Albers uses the water example as an analogy to describe the relative nature of our visual experience of color. Albers writes:
“In the same way as haptic sensations deceive us, so optical illusions deceive.”
Ming and I also see sensory experiences of our world as inescapably relative. We think using an energy yardstick, as Albers did in the above experiment, to assess sensory relationships can be helpful. Many sensory experiences of our world are fundamentally related to energy assessments. Our theory describes the experience of visual relationships as intertwined with a perception of relative energy relationships. Blue light carries a different frequency and energy level than red light.
When our forever-ago ancient ancestors first developed the specialized cell structures which eventually evolved to eyes, they were pursuing an ability to interact with a very basic energy aspect of our world: a perception of light energy. The Energy Theory of Color describes our perception of color as an experience of energy differences in our world. We postulate that as ouyr ancestors evolved, eyes evolved to ascertain energy differences. Along with the perception of luminence (darks and lights), we see the perception of color as an evolutionary adaptation helping to organize our visual world. Our idea is that to see this perception as connected and shaped by energy relationships (not unlike our perception of the water of varying temperatures in the Albers’ experiment described above) is helpful, useful, and, above all, interesting.
This is a demonstration of our theory at work. Small, rapid eye movement, a phenomena called micro-cicades, are what stimulate our optic nerves, especially the cone cells involved in color perception, to send the nerve signals to our brains which help us to "see" color. Without eye movement, we will see no color.
Ming writes:
Matt adds:
The Gelb effect demonstrates that our perception of dark/light relationships are also relative, just as are our perception of colors. You can get an interactive sense of this by following this interactive link.
Color Constancy describes how our brains input relational information, especially clues related to light and light sources, in our assessment of color relationships. In the above example the squares with the arrows appear very different in their color hue . . .
but actually they are the same color!
This is an example drawn from Albers' Interaction of Color.
If you fixate on the black dot in the image on the left for 10 seconds or so, and then move your gaze to the black dot on the right, you will experience an after image that is a complement in color sensation to the image on the left.
We have more to work out in our assessment of sensory fading, but currently we postulate that a view of our visual senses as primarily involved in assessing energy relationships would explain that our eyes will "make up for" and "compensate for" visual stimuli, especially the energy relationships of visual stimuli, in the same way that our tactile senses are shaped by sensations of warm or cold in Albers' example of the three pots of water described at the top of this page.
Do you remember this image going viral on the internet some years back?
Some saw the dress as blue with gold bands and some saw it as white with black bands. Once you joined one or the other of the two camps it seemed very difficult to imagine how folks could see it otherwise.
Ming has explained an explanation of the difference in how we see the colors in this photo can be found in color constancy. Our brains have made differing assumptions about the light source, assumptions our conscious minds are not aware of.
Still curious?
It turns out whether we see the dress is blue or white is directed by whether we see the dress as backlit or in direct light. These assumptions have to do with subtle cues in the photo that are ambiguous. Our brains make this determination without telling us we have made this choice. To find out more you'll need to participate in one or more of our talks, or buy our book, when it comes out!
Copyright © 2024 The Energy Theory of Color - All Rights Reserved.
Powered by GoDaddy
We use cookies to analyze website traffic and optimize your website experience. By accepting our use of cookies, your data will be aggregated with all other user data.